12-27-2007, 02:20 PM
Harvard Magazine
Are some experiences so horrific that the human brain seals them away, only to recall them years later? The concept of ââ¬Årepressed memory,ââ¬Â known by the diagnostic term dissociative amnesia, has long fueled controversy in psychiatry. During the 1980s, claims of childhood sexual abuse based on recovered memories led to a spate of highly publicized court cases. A number of the supposed victims retracted their allegations in the early 1990s, admitting that they had been swayed by therapeutic techniques. Yet the scientific validity of dissociative amnesia has remained contested ground.
In a recent study, professor of psychiatry Harrison Pope, co-director of the Biological Psychiatry Lab at Harvard-affiliated McLean Hospital, put ââ¬Årepressed memoryââ¬Â to the test of time. He reasoned that if dissociative amnesia were an innate capability of the brainââ¬âakin to depression, hallucinations, anxiety, and dementiaââ¬âit would appear in written works throughout history. In collaboration with associate professor of psychiatry James Hudson, Michael Parker, a professor of English at the U.S. Naval Academy, Michael Poliakoff, director of education programs at the National Endowment for the Humanities, and research assistant Matthew Boynes, Pope set out to find the earliest recorded example of a ââ¬Årepressed memory.ââ¬Â
The survey yielded various nineteenth-century instances: best known were A Tale of Two Cities (1859), by Charles Dickens, in which Dr. Manette forgets that he is a physician after his incarceration in the Bastille, and Captains Courageous (1896), by Rudyard Kipling, in which ââ¬ÅPenn,ââ¬Â a former minister, loses his memory after his family perishes in a flood and recalls that trauma only after being involved in a collision at sea. But the survey turned up no examples from pre-modern sources.
The researchers then offered a $1,000 rewardââ¬âposted in three languages on more than 30 Internet websites and discussion groupsââ¬âto the first person to identify a case of dissociative amnesia in any work of fiction or nonfiction prior to 1800. They received more than 100 responses, but none met the ââ¬Årepressed memoryââ¬Â criteria. Although many early texts describe ordinary forgetfulness caused by natural biological processes, as well as instances of individuals forgetting happy memories and even their own identities, there were no accounts of an inability to recall a traumatic experience at one point and the subsequent recovery of that memory.
In a report of their findings published in Psychological Medicine, Pope and his colleagues concluded that the absence of dissociative amnesia in works prior to 1800 indicates that the phenomenon is not a natural neurological function, but rather a ââ¬Åculture-boundââ¬Â syndrome rooted in the nineteenth century. They argued that dissociative amnesia falls into the diagnostic category ââ¬Åpseudo-neurological symptomââ¬Â (or ââ¬Åconversion disorderââ¬Â)ââ¬âa condition that ââ¬Ålacks a recognizable medical or neurological basis.ââ¬Â
The authors have also refuted a number of alternative hypotheses that might explain their survey results. For instance, they argued, the fact that pre-nineteenth- century societies may have conceptualized memory differently than we do cannot account for the lack of recorded descriptions of dissociative amnesia. ââ¬ÅOur ancestors had little understanding about delusions and hallucinations,ââ¬Â Pope points out. ââ¬ÅThey didnââ¬â¢t know about dopamine in the brain or things we now know cause paranoia or auditory hallucinations, but descriptions of hallucinations [appear] in literature for hundreds of years and from all over the world.ââ¬Â Similarly, ââ¬ÅIf an otherwise lucid individual spontaneously develops complete amnesia for a serious traumatic event, such as being raped or witnessing the death of relations or friends,ââ¬Â the researchers explained, ââ¬Åa description of such a case would surely be recognizable, even through a dense veil of cultural interpretationââ¬Â such as spirit possession or some other supernatural event.
What, then, accounts for ââ¬Årepressed memoryââ¬â¢sââ¬Â appearance in the nineteenth century and its endurance today? Pope and his colleagues hope to answer these questions in the future. ââ¬ÅClearly the rise of Romanticism, at the end of the Enlightenment, created fertile soil for the idea that the mind could expunge a trauma from consciousness,ââ¬Â Pope says. He notes that other pseudo-neurological symptoms (such as the female ââ¬Åswoonââ¬Â) emerged during this era, but faded relatively quickly. He suspects that two major factors helped solidify ââ¬Årepressed memoryââ¬Â in the twentieth-century imagination: psychoanalysis (with its theories of the unconscious) and Hollywood. ââ¬ÅFilm is a perfect medium for the idea of repressed memory,ââ¬Â he says. ââ¬ÅThink of the ââ¬Ëflashback,ââ¬â¢ in which a whole childhood trauma is suddenly recalled. Itââ¬â¢s an ideal dramatic device.ââ¬Â
Shortly after publication of their paper, the investigators awarded the $1,000 prize to the nominator of Nina, an opera by Dalayrac and Marsollier performed in Paris in 1786. (Forgetting that she saw her lover apparently lying dead after a duel, the heroine waits for him daily at an appointed spot. When the young man reappears, Nina first seems to recognize him, then doubts his identity, and only slowly accepts him for who he is.) Pope says he and his colleagues were a few years off their threshold of 1800, but he believes their argument holds: ââ¬ÅThe challenge falls upon anyone who believes that repressed memory is real to explain its absence for thousands of years.ââ¬Â
Are some experiences so horrific that the human brain seals them away, only to recall them years later? The concept of ââ¬Årepressed memory,ââ¬Â known by the diagnostic term dissociative amnesia, has long fueled controversy in psychiatry. During the 1980s, claims of childhood sexual abuse based on recovered memories led to a spate of highly publicized court cases. A number of the supposed victims retracted their allegations in the early 1990s, admitting that they had been swayed by therapeutic techniques. Yet the scientific validity of dissociative amnesia has remained contested ground.
In a recent study, professor of psychiatry Harrison Pope, co-director of the Biological Psychiatry Lab at Harvard-affiliated McLean Hospital, put ââ¬Årepressed memoryââ¬Â to the test of time. He reasoned that if dissociative amnesia were an innate capability of the brainââ¬âakin to depression, hallucinations, anxiety, and dementiaââ¬âit would appear in written works throughout history. In collaboration with associate professor of psychiatry James Hudson, Michael Parker, a professor of English at the U.S. Naval Academy, Michael Poliakoff, director of education programs at the National Endowment for the Humanities, and research assistant Matthew Boynes, Pope set out to find the earliest recorded example of a ââ¬Årepressed memory.ââ¬Â
The survey yielded various nineteenth-century instances: best known were A Tale of Two Cities (1859), by Charles Dickens, in which Dr. Manette forgets that he is a physician after his incarceration in the Bastille, and Captains Courageous (1896), by Rudyard Kipling, in which ââ¬ÅPenn,ââ¬Â a former minister, loses his memory after his family perishes in a flood and recalls that trauma only after being involved in a collision at sea. But the survey turned up no examples from pre-modern sources.
The researchers then offered a $1,000 rewardââ¬âposted in three languages on more than 30 Internet websites and discussion groupsââ¬âto the first person to identify a case of dissociative amnesia in any work of fiction or nonfiction prior to 1800. They received more than 100 responses, but none met the ââ¬Årepressed memoryââ¬Â criteria. Although many early texts describe ordinary forgetfulness caused by natural biological processes, as well as instances of individuals forgetting happy memories and even their own identities, there were no accounts of an inability to recall a traumatic experience at one point and the subsequent recovery of that memory.
In a report of their findings published in Psychological Medicine, Pope and his colleagues concluded that the absence of dissociative amnesia in works prior to 1800 indicates that the phenomenon is not a natural neurological function, but rather a ââ¬Åculture-boundââ¬Â syndrome rooted in the nineteenth century. They argued that dissociative amnesia falls into the diagnostic category ââ¬Åpseudo-neurological symptomââ¬Â (or ââ¬Åconversion disorderââ¬Â)ââ¬âa condition that ââ¬Ålacks a recognizable medical or neurological basis.ââ¬Â
The authors have also refuted a number of alternative hypotheses that might explain their survey results. For instance, they argued, the fact that pre-nineteenth- century societies may have conceptualized memory differently than we do cannot account for the lack of recorded descriptions of dissociative amnesia. ââ¬ÅOur ancestors had little understanding about delusions and hallucinations,ââ¬Â Pope points out. ââ¬ÅThey didnââ¬â¢t know about dopamine in the brain or things we now know cause paranoia or auditory hallucinations, but descriptions of hallucinations [appear] in literature for hundreds of years and from all over the world.ââ¬Â Similarly, ââ¬ÅIf an otherwise lucid individual spontaneously develops complete amnesia for a serious traumatic event, such as being raped or witnessing the death of relations or friends,ââ¬Â the researchers explained, ââ¬Åa description of such a case would surely be recognizable, even through a dense veil of cultural interpretationââ¬Â such as spirit possession or some other supernatural event.
What, then, accounts for ââ¬Årepressed memoryââ¬â¢sââ¬Â appearance in the nineteenth century and its endurance today? Pope and his colleagues hope to answer these questions in the future. ââ¬ÅClearly the rise of Romanticism, at the end of the Enlightenment, created fertile soil for the idea that the mind could expunge a trauma from consciousness,ââ¬Â Pope says. He notes that other pseudo-neurological symptoms (such as the female ââ¬Åswoonââ¬Â) emerged during this era, but faded relatively quickly. He suspects that two major factors helped solidify ââ¬Årepressed memoryââ¬Â in the twentieth-century imagination: psychoanalysis (with its theories of the unconscious) and Hollywood. ââ¬ÅFilm is a perfect medium for the idea of repressed memory,ââ¬Â he says. ââ¬ÅThink of the ââ¬Ëflashback,ââ¬â¢ in which a whole childhood trauma is suddenly recalled. Itââ¬â¢s an ideal dramatic device.ââ¬Â
Shortly after publication of their paper, the investigators awarded the $1,000 prize to the nominator of Nina, an opera by Dalayrac and Marsollier performed in Paris in 1786. (Forgetting that she saw her lover apparently lying dead after a duel, the heroine waits for him daily at an appointed spot. When the young man reappears, Nina first seems to recognize him, then doubts his identity, and only slowly accepts him for who he is.) Pope says he and his colleagues were a few years off their threshold of 1800, but he believes their argument holds: ââ¬ÅThe challenge falls upon anyone who believes that repressed memory is real to explain its absence for thousands of years.ââ¬Â